The Elaboration Likelihood Model
The Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) is a theoretical framework in social psychology that explains how people process and respond to persuasive messages. Developed by Richard Petty and John Cacioppo in 1981, the ELM posits that there are two routes to persuasion: the central route and the peripheral route. The central route involves cognitive elaboration, or thoughtful consideration of the message content, while the peripheral route involves less cognitive effort and relies on superficial cues.
The ELM proposes that the level of elaboration and the route of processing depend on the receiver’s motivation and ability to process the message (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). According to the model, when individuals are motivated and able to process a message, they are more likely to engage in cognitive elaboration and use the central route of persuasion. On the other hand, when individuals are less motivated or have limited cognitive resources, they are more likely to rely on peripheral cues, such as the speaker’s credibility, attractiveness, or emotional appeals.
The ELM has been widely applied in the fields of marketing, advertising, and public communication, as it provides a framework for understanding how persuasive messages can be designed and tailored to be more effective. For example, messages that target highly motivated and able audiences should focus on providing strong arguments and evidence to persuade them, while messages that target less motivated or less able audiences should use more peripheral cues, such as celebrity endorsements or emotional appeals (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986).
Empirical research has supported the ELM’s predictions, showing that the central route leads to more enduring and resistant attitudes, while the peripheral route leads to more temporary and superficial attitudes (Petty & Cacioppo, 1986). Additionally, studies have shown that individual differences, such as personality traits and prior knowledge, can influence the likelihood of elaboration and the route of processing (Petty et al., 1995).
Despite its usefulness, the ELM has also been criticized for its oversimplification of the persuasion process and its failure to account for the interplay between the central and peripheral routes (Chaiken, 1987). Some scholars have argued that persuasion is a more complex and dynamic process that involves multiple factors, including the receiver’s emotional state, the context, and the source characteristics (Chaiken, 1987).
In conclusion, the Elaboration Likelihood Model is a theoretical framework that explains how persuasive messages can be processed and responded to by receivers. It posits that there are two routes to persuasion, the central route and the peripheral route, and that the level of elaboration and the route of processing depend on the receiver’s motivation and ability to process the message. Despite its limitations, the ELM has provided a useful framework for understanding and designing persuasive messages in various domains.
The Elaboration Likelihood Model (ELM) has been extensively used as a theoretical framework in advertising research to understand how consumers process and respond to persuasive messages in advertisements. The model has been applied in various domains of advertising, including print ads, television commercials, and online ads.
One study conducted by Petty and Cacioppo (1984) used the ELM to examine the effects of message complexity on attitudes towards a product. The study found that highly motivated and able individuals were more influenced by strong arguments in a complex message, whereas less motivated and less able individuals were more influenced by peripheral cues such as the attractiveness of the spokesperson.
Another study by Chaiken and Maheswaran (1994) used the ELM to investigate the impact of source credibility on persuasion in advertising. The study found that highly credible sources were more effective in persuading individuals who were highly involved in the issue being advertised, while less credible sources were more effective in persuading individuals who were less involved.
References
Chaiken, S. (1987). The heuristic model of persuasion. In M. P. Zanna, J. M. Olson, & C. P. Herman (Eds.), Social influence: The Ontario Symposium (Vol. 5, pp. 3-39). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Chaiken, S., & Maheswaran, D. (1994). Heuristic processing can bias systematic processing: Effects of source credibility, argument ambiguity, and task importance on attitude judgment. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 66(3), 460-473.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1986). Communication and persuasion: Central and peripheral routes to attitude change. New York: Springer-Verlag.
Petty, R. E., & Cacioppo, J. T. (1984). The effects of involvement on responses to argument quantity and quality: Central and peripheral routes to persuasion. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 46(1), 69-81.
Petty, R. E., Tormala, Z. L., Briñol, P., & Jarvis, W. B. G. (1995). Implicit ambivalence from attitude change: An exploration of the PAST model. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 68(4), 597-612.